
Megaminds
The search for the world’s best memory athletes
SCIENCE | Jessica Lussenhop
A man in a fedora walks into an empty classroom on the second floor of a building on Washington University’s campus and waits in the darkened room. He has never been here before – not to this classroom, not even to St. Louis.
The professor arrives and flicks on a light.
Soon students dressed in the 8 a.m. class uniform of track pants and sweatshirts begin to trickle through the door. As each enters the room, the fedora man walks over and introduces himself with a handshake. Before long the entire room is a din of chatter and tapping laptop keys. There are roughly 40 students assembled.
The professor asks for their attention. But instead of beginning his lecture, he steps back, and the man in the fedora stands up.
“Hi, everyone. My name is Chester Santos. I am the 2008 USA National Memory Champion,” he says, walking to the front of the room. “If you remember meeting me when you walked in today, if I shook your hand, gave you my name, got your name, if you remember that happening, please stand up.”
Chair legs scrape as the majority of the room rises.
“John?” Santos asks, pointing to a shaggy student closest to the far wall.
John nods and sits. “Charlotte,” Santos continues, starting with the girl at the front of the row next to John. He goes to the boy behind her, “Dan.”
This story was originally published in Riverfront Times, St. Louis’ news and arts weekly. An abridged version is reprinted here with permission. Copyright Riverfront Times 2012.
Santos starts to pick up speed. “Helene. Sophie. Keisha. Vivienne. Nick.
Jackie. Ashley. Prateek. Ashley. Ben. John. Neil. Huh,” he pauses at an Asian girl about halfway through the room. “I’m not 100 percent positive on you. I’ll come back.”
He continues: “Alec. Shane. Isaiah. Rohit.
Marlow. Emily. Laura. Deena. Talia. Jeremy. Moe. Anna. Reggie. Jessica. Wayan. Gaithree. Rosie.
“And then you,” he says swinging back around to the lone girl standing in the middle of the room, “it’s Kim.”
“Yes,” she nods. “Wow,” someone murmurs, and the room breaks into applause.
After meeting them for mere seconds – jetlagged and not necessarily in the best “memory shape” of his life – Santos correctly names 33 students. In the past he’s successfully made it through an auditorium of 200. It’s not just a quick one-off, either. As the floor opens up to questions, he calls on each person by name.
The year he took the title, Santos memorized a 135-digit sequence, and studied and recited the order of a pack of playing cards in a minute and 27 seconds. At a public event in March, he named all 535 men and women of the U.S. Congress as well as their district number.
“I’ve gotten better and better and better,” he tells the students.
After the demo is over, Santos’ handler walks him down a flight of stairs and outside toward the Psychology Building next door, where for the last three days he has been through a battery of tests designed to unlock the secrets of his brain. Some of the challenges feel familiar – recognizing names and faces, memorizing word lists – but others are intended to probe his limitations.
“My prediction,” Santos says, confident after his second day of testing, “is we’re still going to perform above average.”
By
“we,” Santos is referring to his fellow “memory athletes.” In the
United States there are roughly 50 active competitors, called “MAs” for
short; in places like the United Kingdom, Germany and China there are
hundreds more. In meets all over the globe these men and women converge
to outthink one another in a series of mental tasks designed to test the
limits of human memory capacity. Records are broken at these events
every year; some believe the brain’s powers of recall are limitless. But
if there is a ceiling, the memory athletes will hit it.
He doesn’t know it at the time, but Santos’ boast about his fellow MAs turns out to be prescient.
“These
people are just remarkable,” says Henry “Roddy” Roediger III, a doctor
of psychology, who, as part of Wash. U.’s Superior Memory Project, has
invited the world’s foremost megaminds to St. Louis. “Almost every test
we give the memory athletes, even though they’ve never taken these tests
before.... They do better.”
TEST IS BEST
Roediger
– a more-salt-than-pepper-haired 65-year-old with a slightly crooked
smile – rose to prominence in the 1990s when he entered the infamous
“memory wars” at a time when therapy patients all over the nation were
acquiring “recovered memories” through controversial techniques such as
hypnosis. Visions of horrific sexual abuse and Satanic rituals were
assumed to be true. Dozens of cases went to court. The “Satanic panic”
died down after investigations proved in some cases that the abuses
couldn’t have happened. Roediger influenced the conversation by showing
that if given a list of words (bed, rest, awake, tired, dream), subjects reported they’d seen related words that never appeared on the list (such as sleep or slumber).
The finding proved that it was possible to create false memories.
“That
is among one of the most widely cited works in the field of memory,”
says Elizabeth Loftus, a professor of law and psychology and a memory
expert at University of California-Irvine. “He is a master at designing
and conducting experiments.”
In
the last 10 years Roediger’s research into the “testing effect” proved
that – as unpopular as the notion may be with students – repeated,
low-stakes test-taking helps pupils retain information better than
simply “studying” the material for the same amount of time. The theory
played out when Roediger and his students implemented it in seventh- and
eighth-grade classes at Columbia Middle School in Illinois.
“What
he’s found is, the best way to make a memory is to retrieve the
information. It has enormous practical implications,” says James
McGaugh, a neuroscientist and superiormemory researcher at UC-Irvine.
“It’s solely his discovery.”
Now
Roediger, his frequent collaborator Kathleen McDermott, and David
Balota, a cognitive-psychology professor at Wash. U., hope that studying
memory athletes will unravel what Roediger believes could be a
particularly helpful form of superior memory – one that’s conceivably
accessible to anyone.
“We
hope, by studying how it succeeds, that maybe all of us can improve our
memories,” says Roediger. “Hopefully we can find some things that help
in remediating memory for people who have impaired memory.”
Memory workout
Nelson Dellis will never forget the two of hearts and the ten of diamonds.
Sitting in the hot seat onstage in the final round of the USA Memory Championship in 2010, Dellis appeared collected.
To his left sat the defending champion, and next to him, the 2005
titleholder. All that separated Dellis, a lanky, six-foot-seven-inch
University of Miami graduate student, from the championship were two
packs of playing cards.
The
competitors had survived a gauntlet of memory tasks in order to get to
this point. That morning they were tasked with memorizing 117 faces and
names, a list of 500 digits, 100 words, a previously unpublished poem
and the order of a deck of cards. Dellis had already bested both of his
final competitors in speed numbers and memorizing cards. He figured he
had it in the bag.
For
the final event all three men memorized the same two decks and were
about to take turns in a sudden-death elimination challenge, naming the
order of the cards one at a time. Dellis went first.
“Ten
of diamonds,” he said confidently. There was a tiny silence. One of the
other memory athletes shook his head, making a “cut-it” motion with his
hand.
“Nelson, we’ve got two of hearts,” the moderator responded.
Dellis’
eyes flew to the moderator, then to the ceiling. He’d named the last
card in the deck, not the first. As the round proceeded without him, he
looked into the audience and smiled mirthlessly at his girlfriend.
“Everyone who was supporting me was really disappointed,” he recalls. “I thought I was going to win.”
The experience highlights another trick of human memory – we recall tragedy better than triumph.
Though as a spectator sport the USA Memory Championship leaves
something to be desired, this is where superior memories are forged.
Most memory athletes swear that anyone can do what they do and, though
highly competitive, they’re willing to share their tricks.
The lessons, however, can be awkward.
Seated
in the lounge of Wash. U.’s Olin Library, Santos cringes slightly when
this reporter presents him with a photo of a female and asks him to
explain how he would remember the woman’s name.
“You’re not going to tell her, are you?” he asks.
The
woman in the photo is named “Dana,” and after a quick glance – Santos
says he needs about five seconds to lock in a name with a face – he has
it.
“OK, so, when I
saw her, first thing that came to my mind is that she has a pretty long
pointy nose,” says Santos. “So you want to exaggerate it and see it as a
really long nose, and I’d stick a Danish to it.”
The
Pinocchio nose spearing a Danish would likely be enough for Santos to
get “Dana.” But if he needed some extra help he might envision an apple
exploding out of the Danish dangling from the tip of the nose – a
reminder of the final ‘a’ sound in “Dana.”
“Next time I see her I would immediately notice the nose, and all that crazy imagery comes back to me,” he says.
Earlier
that same afternoon, Brad Zupp – a wiry, bespectacled Upstate New
Yorker who works as a full-time juggler and magician – paints a
similarly graphic tableau for memorizing playing cards. He’s fond of a
technique called “person-action-object,” which allows him to remember
groups of three cards in short sentences.
Zupp’s wife is using a scissors to excitedly open a Christmas present.
This
sentence corresponds to the queen of hearts, three of clubs and ace of
spades. Zupp has a person or a character assigned to all 52 playing
cards. Like many memory athletes, the queen of hearts is someone of
great emotional significance – his wife. He’s assigned his hairdresser
to the three of clubs (signified by scissors) and Santa Claus to the ace
of spades (represented by the present she’s ripping apart).
“They’re
pictures, so I don’t have to keep thinking about them to keep them
there,” he explains. “I’m not using repetition, I’m using images and
picturing them and making some really silly and creative associations.”
These
mnemonic memorization systems are well known to Roediger. He likes
using a primitive one to psych out his students by flawlessly
memorizing, after just one try, a list of 20 words forward and backward
on the first day of class.
“That really blew people’s minds,” he says. “The tricks are ancient. The Greeks and Romans knew about them.”
In 2002 a brain-imaging study done on competitors from the
World Memory Championship in London showed no physical differences in
the memory athletes’ brains, though some areas associated with
navigation lit up as they interpreted information as pictures and
scenes. This seemingly bolsters the memory athletes’ insistence that
anyone can do what they do, but the researchers want to know more.
“First
what we have to do is have a good cognitive profile of these
individuals,” says Balota. “Is it the case that they just have general
better cognitive skills, or is it the case that they’re really truly
outstanding consolidators?”
Can anybody do it? 
There
are no MRI scanners or cranial electrodes in the cognitive lab on the
third floor of Wash. U.’s Psychology Building, only a basket full of
mini Twix bars at the front desk and a row of private rooms with
computers.
The MAs are put through
three days of testing, much of it consisting of staring at monitors.
Though Roediger is still far from having publishable data, striking
results are already emerging from just a quick glance at the test
results of five memory athletes, including Santos, Zupp and Dellis.
In
one test, called the Stroop Color-Word Test, the mental athletes are
shown a list of words in a colored font. When a word is printed, for
example, in green ink, but the word itself says “red,” most test-takers
show some amount of “interference.” They’re tripped up by the word and
answer incorrectly when asked what color the word was written in.
Conversely, if a neutral word like “chair” is written in blue ink,
there’s no trouble at all in naming the correct color.
The memory athletes, however, do not appear to fall for this trap.
“They show about half as much interference in the color-word test,” Roediger says.
The
tasks also reveal that the athletes’ memories are not superhuman, nor
somehow “photographic.” In another test, the athletes memorize
“non-words” – pure gibberish. The researchers asked this reporter not to
reprint the exact words found on the task, but imagine trying to
memorize these words: flerp, conternsta, utopare, yerf, orniga, palkemf.
Most people just give up. And at first, the memory athletes do about as poorly as average test takers.
That
is, until they’re presented with a list like that again. Some of the
athletes reported that they switched strategies. Dellis boasts that on
his test he invented meanings for the words – as if composing the
fictional Game of Thrones language Dothraki on the fly. And it worked.
“That
was really hard,” says Dellis, who, after that devastating loss at the
2010 USA Memory Championship, came back to win it in 2011 and is now the
two-time reigning national champion. “Everybody has a very steep dip in
the scores for that. Mine were still pretty good.”
Though
the results are far from definitive, K. Anders Ericsson, a Florida
State University cognitive psychologist who’s been studying memory
athletes for decades, found it similarly difficult to “interfere” with
the memory of Chao Lu, the man who holds the world record for memorized
digits of pi (a staggering 67,890), by setting up tests intended to
distract or interfere with his mnemonic concentration.
“These manipulations,” Ericsson wrote, “did not reduce Chao Lu’s virtually perfect accuracy of recall.”
It
is promising results such these that are an endless source of
frustration to the memory athletes and the founders of the USA Memory
Championship: The techniques they espouse aren’t taught in schools.
“If
you can get into this and do it through high school and college and
through law school – if I would have known it back then, it would’ve
changed my life,” says Dellis. “I think it should be taught to kids, for
sure.”
But Ericsson
does raise an important point. “From my experience, people who would be
interested in training their memory are not your average person,” he
says.
Roediger puts it
even more bluntly: “You’re not going to take somebody who’s kind of a
dullard – they’re not going to be memorizing a deck of cards in 24 and a
half seconds.”
Whether
or not the memory techniques can truly soup up the mind’s faculties is a
question that a second phase of Roediger’s project could attempt to
answer, using students. There is talk within the Superior Memory Project
team of starting a freshman seminar in fall 2013 that would not only
teach about the way mnemonics work, but attempt to turn its participants
into the next generation of memory champions. The researchers would
then track the students’ academic progress for the remainder of their time at Wash. U.
“We’d
compare them to other freshmen who are taking random [seminars]. We’d
measure them all beforehand and make sure there’s no difference,”
Roediger says. “It’s a natural experiment. We can just follow my 20, or
however many, from my freshman seminar. Do they make better grades?”
Memory mountaineer
In his
stately office, the bookshelves piled to the ceiling with titles
promising to improve one’s memory, Roediger concedes he’s noticed signs
of his own memory faltering.
“I
do have name-finding difficulty. That’s the first thing to go in older
adults,” he says. “Strangely, it is restaurants. I’ll say, ‘Let’s go to
somewhere.’ I’ll have it perfectly envisioned in my mind and then not be
able to come up with a name. It’s annoying.”
This
is, of course, where the two disciplines of studying memory success and
memory failure meet. The dream would ultimately be to save us from, not
just the day-to-day perils of forgetting to buy milk or putting a
toilet seat down, but from memory deterioration due to accidents,
illness and age.
Roediger
doesn’t get sentimental about his own memories, nor grandiose about
whether his research will be able to stave off his or anyone else’s
memory loss. As for the memory athletes themselves, their ambitions for
their skills range quite a bit.
Brad
Zupp incorporated teaching the techniques into his act long ago, with
the hope that kids who struggle with rote memorization in school will
find his methods helpful. Chester Santos bills himself as “The
International Man of Memory” and travels the globe giving talks to
corporations and business leaders on how to make flawless presentations
without notes, or remember important people after just one meeting.
Nelson Dellis does one-onone coaching and is the spokesperson for two
memory-related companies (health supplements and data servers), but
dreams of one day being commissioned by professional sports teams to
help, say, a new NFL quarterback memorize his Bible-thick stack of
plays.
He has also
started a charity that marries his two passions – memory training and
mountaineering. It’s called Climb for Memory, and it seeks to raise
awareness and funds for studying Alzheimer’s. As he has often remarked,
Dellis began pursuing memory training in the first place because of his
own grandmother’s descent into dementia.
“I wanted to see if there was anything I could do for my own mind,” he says.
Roediger
says there’s a controversial theory that supposes that building up
one’s skill as a memorizer, or “cognitive reserve,” could stave off the
outward signs of dementia – that it’s worthwhile to be an ant
stockpiling brain cells for winter while the rest of the grasshoppers
sing. But it’s totally unproven and, as of today, none of the world’s
memory champions is old enough to prove that their cerebral stockpile is
keeping them free of dementia.
Roediger
sounds skeptical but cautiously open-minded to the idea that Dellis, or
anyone else, could save himself from such a fate.
“That would be the great hope,” says Roediger. “Maybe he will. Nobody knows.”
Jessica Lussenhop is a staff writer at the Riverfront Times in St. Louis; previously she held that same position at City Pages, RFT’s sister paper in the Twin Cities. Find her on Twitter @ Lussenpop.